SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS OF AGGRESSION

t

Social psychological explanations of human aggression include the frustration-aggression hypothesis, social learning theory as applied to human aggression, and de-individuation

SOCIAL LEARNING THEORY:

INTRODUCTION

Social learning theory is the behaviour theory most relevant to criminology. Albert Bandura's theory suggests that violent propensities are not an inherent part of human nature but are instead acquired through a process known as behavioural modelling. This theory posits that individuals often emulate behaviours exhibited by figures they respect or admire, particularly when these behaviours appear to be rewarded or reinforced. Such reinforcements can manifest in various forms, such as alleviating stress, acquiring material gains, or enhancing social standing and self-esteem.

According to Bandura, influential models for behaviour can be found in an immediate social circle and broader cultural and media contexts. A notable example of this phenomenon is the replication of aggressive behaviours within familial settings. Children who observe aggressive interactions, particularly those involving close family members, are more likely to adopt similar methods of conflict resolution. For instance, witnessing a father's abusive behaviour towards a mother might predispose a child to exhibit similar behaviours in adulthood, underscoring the impact of adult behaviour on a child's developmental trajectory. Bandura also emphasized the significance of early identification and intervention in managing aggression. By recognizing and addressing aggressive tendencies during childhood, it may be possible to steer individuals away from potential criminal activities in later life. In essence, Bandura’s social learning theory provides a framework for understanding aggression by examining The mechanisms through which aggressive behavioural patterns are formed. The catalysts that initiate aggressive responses in individuals. The determinants influence the persistence or cessation of aggressive behaviours in subsequent contexts. Bandura's observational learning theory outlines four crucial processes that influence an observer’s behaviour after exposure to a model.

These processes are attention, retention, motor reproduction, and motivation.

Attention: This is the foundational step in observational learning. An individual must first notice and focus on the key aspects of the modelled behaviour. For instance, in the context of aggression, children must observe closely what the aggressor is doing and saying to imitate that behaviour effectively. The Bobo doll experiment demonstrated this, where children observed live or filmed models verbally and physically abusing a Bobo doll.

Retention: After paying attention to the behaviour, the individual must be able to remember it. This involves encoding the observed behaviour into long-term memory for later retrieval and, for example, recalling a specific aggressive action seen earlier. The Bobo doll experiment showed that children could retain and later replicate the aggressive behaviours they had observed, indicating that these actions had been successfully stored in their memory.

Motor Reproduction: This process involves the observer’s physical ability to replicate the behaviour. After learning behaviour through attention and retention, the individual must have the physical capability to perform it. For instance, children in the Bobo doll experiment had the physical ability to mimic the actions they had seen, such as hitting the doll.

Motivation: The final component is the expectation of rewards or reinforcements for replicating the modelled behaviour. In the Bobo doll experiment, children saw adults receiving vicarious reinforcement for their aggression, prompting them to emulate these actions in anticipation of similar rewards.

This concept extends to broader societal contexts, where violent acts may be glorified or rewarded, as seen in the historical glorification of figures like Bonnie and Clyde. Bandura suggested that such media portrayals can influence children to replicate aggressive behaviours, with long-term impacts on their personality and a potential inclination towards violence in adulthood. These four processes explain how observational learning can lead to acquiring and replicating behaviours, including aggression, influenced by immediate observations and broader societal messages.

Albert Bandura's research on social learning theory highlights the significant role of environmental experiences in shaping children's learning of violent behaviours. He posited that exposure to violence, whether in one's immediate environment or through media, can lead to increased aggression in children. Here's a summary of the key points: Influence of High Crime Environments: Bandura observed that individuals living in areas with high crime rates are likelier to exhibit violent behaviour than those in low-crime areas. This observation suggests that environmental factors play a crucial role in the social learning of violence. Impact of Television and Media: Bandura identified television and films as powerful sources of behaviour modelling. Violent acts portrayed in media, especially when committed by 'heroes' who face no consequences, can be internalized as acceptable by viewers.

This is particularly impactful for children, who may be more impressionable. The frequent depiction of aggression in media can lead to children imitating these aggressive behaviours. Examples of Media Influenced Violence: Several real-world incidents underscore the potential influence of media violence on behaviour. For instance, David Phillips noted a spike in homicide rates following televised heavyweight boxing matches. The attempted assassination of President Ronald Reagan by John Hinckley, who was reportedly influenced by the movie "Taxi Driver," and the replication of a violent act from the film "Born Innocent" in a real-life assault case are examples that highlight the potential impact of media violence.

BOBO DOLL EXPERIMENT APFC Albert Bandura's Bobo doll experiment is a seminal study in psychology, particularly in understanding how children learn and imitate aggressive behaviours. Here's a detailed overview of the experiment: AIMS Primary Objective: Bandura aimed to demonstrate that children would imitate aggressive behaviour witnessed by an adult model. This was based on his hypothesis that aggression in children is learned through observation and imitation.

METHODOLOGY Participants: The study involved 72 children (36 boys and 36 girls) aged between 37 and 69 months, with a mean age of 4 and 4 months. Role Models: One male adult and one female adult were models for aggressive behaviour. Pre-Existing Aggressiveness Assessment: To ensure a balanced and fair experiment, children were matched based on their levels of aggression. This assessment was done by observing the children in a nursery setting and rating their aggressive behaviour using four 5-point scales. Matched Pairs Design: This approach ensured that children with similar levels of aggression were placed in each experimental group, making it a controlled and balanced study.

PROCEDURES: Positive Reinforcement Condition: In this scenario, children observed an adult model being rewarded (or receiving positive reinforcement) for aggressive behaviour towards the Bobo doll. Punishment Condition: The adult model was punished for displaying aggressive behaviour towards the Bobo doll. This condition was intended to study the effects of negative consequences on imitating aggression. Control/Neutral Group: This group served as a baseline, where children were not exposed to any specific model behaviour towards the Bobo doll.

KEY FINDINGS: Key findings of the experiment include Higher Aggression in Positive Reinforcement Condition: Children exposed to models rewarded for aggression (positive reinforcement condition) exhibited more aggressive responses than those who saw models punished for aggression. Gender Differences in Aggression: Boys generally showed higher levels of aggression than girls in the positive reinforcement condition.

Notably, boys were more aggressive when the model was male, while girls exhibited more physical aggression when the model was male and more verbal aggression when the model was female. An interesting deviation was observed in how often they punched the Bobo doll, where the gender effects were reversed. Support for Social Learning Theory:

The results support Bandura's Social Learning Theory, which posits that children learn social behaviours like aggression through observational learning, i.e., by watching others. Factors Influencing Imitation of Aggression: Vicarious Reinforcement: Observing a model being rewarded for aggressive behaviour increases the likelihood of the observer imitating that behaviour.

For example, a child is more likely to imitate aggression if they see another child getting their way through aggression. Similarity to the Model: Models similar to the observer (e.g., in terms of gender) are more likely to be imitated. Admiration of the Model's Status: Models admired for their status (e.g., successful personalities like athletes) are more likely to be imitated than less admired figures. Bandura's Bobo doll experiment was a cornerstone in demonstrating the role of observational learning in the development of aggressive behaviour, emphasizing the influence of external models and the conditions under which aggressive behaviour is learned and reproduced.

SIGNIFICANCE OF THE STUDY: Influence on Psychological Theories: The experiment played a crucial role in shaping the understanding of social learning theory, emphasising the importance of observational learning in children. Implications for Media and Aggression: The findings also had broader implications for understanding how children can be influenced by aggression portrayed in media and their environment.

CONCLUSIONS: The findings seem to support Bandura's Social Learning Theory.  Children learn social behaviour such as aggression through observation and watching another person's behaviour. Bandura's series of Bobo doll experiments revealed several factors determining whether observed aggressive behaviour is imitated: Vicarious reinforcement – if the model is observed to use aggression. The outcome is desirable; it increases the likelihood that the behaviour will be imitated, e.g., if a child sees another child get their way by hitting someone, they are more likely to imitate the aggressive behaviour. Models are more likely to be imitated if they are similar to the observer, e.g., of the same sex.

A model is more likely to be imitated if the observer admires their status, e.g., children are more likely to imitate successful football players like David Beckham than less successful players like Wayne Rooney. Bandura's Bobo doll experiment demonstrated that aggressive behaviour could be learned through observation and imitation, a fundamental concept in the study of child development and psychology. The experiment highlighted how children's behaviour can be shaped by the models they observe in their environment, leading to significant discussions and further research in developmental psychology, education, and media studies.

THE ECLECTIC APPROACH To understand aggression, as highlighted in the context of Bandura's experiment, suggests that a single theory, such as Social Learning Theory (SLT), is insufficient to fully explain complex behaviours like aggression. This approach acknowledges the multifaceted nature of human behaviour, considering various factors, including emotions, personality, environment, socioeconomic class, culture, and biological influences. Diversity in Responses: Not all children in Bandura’s experiment reacted similarly, indicating that individual differences play a significant role in behaviour. This variation could be attributed to several factors beyond what SLT explains. For instance, emotional states, personality traits, and environmental influences could account for the differences in how children responded to the Bobo doll. Influence of Genetics and Environment: The research on the MAOA gene (often linked to aggression) illustrates how genetic predispositions interact with environmental factors. Children with the low-activity variant of the MAOA gene (MAOA-L) may only exhibit aggressive behaviour if they experience an abusive environment. This interplay of nature (genetics) and nurture (environment) suggests a more nuanced understanding of aggression, where the environment experiences moderate biological predispositions.

GENDER DIFFERENCES: Bandura’s study showed gender differences in aggressive behaviour, with boys being more aggressive than girls. While SLT might explain this through differential reinforcement and observational learning, biological and evolutionary psychologists might interpret this through the lens of hormonal differences (e.g., testosterone levels) and adaptive behaviours in evolutionary history. This perspective posits that males, due to evolutionary pressures, might be more predisposed to aggressive behaviours for survival and defence. Beta and Alpha Bias: The criticism of beta bias in Bandura's theory points out that it minimises or ignores gender differences. An alpha-biased approach, in contrast, would emphasise and explore these differences, acknowledging that males and females might exhibit different patterns of aggression due to biological, psychological, and social factors. In conclusion, an eclectic approach to understanding aggression underscores the importance of integrating multiple perspectives and theories. By considering biological, psychological, social, and cultural factors, this approach offers a more comprehensive understanding of why individuals may exhibit aggressive behaviour, acknowledging the complexity and diversity of human behaviour.

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES: Social Learning Theory (SLT), proposed by Albert Bandura, emphasises the role of observation, imitation, and modelling in learning behaviour, including aggression. However, SLT does not fully account for instances where individuals who are not normally aggressive exhibit uncharacteristically aggressive behaviour in certain situations. This limitation can be attributed to several factors: Situational Factors: SLT primarily focuses on learning through observation and does not extensively address how specific situations or contexts elicit aggressive behaviour from typically non-aggressive individuals. Situational factors such as stress, provocation, alcohol influence, group dynamics, or anonymity can trigger aggressive responses that are not in line with an individual's usual behaviour. Cognitive and Emotional States: The theory does not sufficiently explain how internal cognitive and emotional states influence aggression. Factors like frustration, anger, fear, or perceived threats can lead to aggressive behaviour inconsistent with an individual's normal conduct. These internal states can momentarily override learned behaviours. Biological and Evolutionary Factors: SLT tends to downplay the role of biological influences on behaviour. Factors like genetic predispositions, hormonal influences (such as testosterone levels), or brain chemistry can play a significant role in aggressive behaviour. These biological aspects can sometimes lead to aggression in typically non-aggressive individuals. Individual Differences: Everyone has unique personal characteristics that influence how they respond to situations. SLT does not fully address these individual differences, which can lead to varying responses to similar environmental stimuli. For example, a person might react aggressively in a situation where others might not due to personal traits or past experiences not accounted for by SLT.

The Complexity of Human Behaviour: Human behaviour is complex and influenced by many factors. While SLT provides a valuable framework for understanding how behaviour is learned, it does not encompass all the variables that can lead to aggressive actions, particularly in atypical situations. In summary, while Social Learning Theory offers important insights into how aggressive behaviour can be learned and modelled, it does not comprehensively explain why individuals who are usually not aggressive might behave aggressively in certain situations. This limitation points to the need for a more holistic approach considering situational, cognitive, emotional, biological, and individual factors in understanding human behaviour.

SLT RESEARCH IS NOT SCIENTIFIC: The inherent limitations of Social Learning Theory (SLT) in conducting scientific research are notable. Since SLT primarily depends on methodologies such as correlations, observations, and questionnaires, it encounters significant challenges in establishing causal relationships. While useful for identifying patterns and associations, these methods do not inherently prove causation. They reveal how two or more variables may travel together, but other factors could be coincidental or influence this relationship. For instance, a correlation between aggressive behaviour and exposure to violent media does not necessarily mean one causes the other; there could be underlying factors influencing both. This issue is akin to the fallacious argument that all beings who drink water will eventually die, which oversimplifies complex relationships. Thus, SLT's reliance on such non-experimental methods necessitates careful interpretation and acknowledgement of their limitations in conclusively determining cause and effect. Here are some key reasons why it is difficult to study aggression scientifically, particularly through the lens of SLT:

DEMAND CHARACTERISTICS: If participants become aware that a study is investigating aggression, they may alter their behaviour in response to what they expect the experimenters to see. This can skew results and make drawing accurate conclusions about naturalistic aggressive behaviours difficult. Control of Variables: Aggressive behaviour is influenced by many factors, including genetic predispositions, family environment, peer influence, cultural norms, media exposure, educational experiences, and even diet. Controlling for all these variables in a research study is virtually impossible, complicating the ability to establish clear cause-and-effect relationships.

METHODOLOGICAL LIMITATIONS: Alternative methods to experimental studies, such as correlational studies, case studies, or interviews, have limitations. For example, while correlational studies can show a relationship between exposure to violence (like TV violence) and aggressive behaviour, they cannot establish causality. Case studies and interviews provide in-depth information but lack generalisability. Unknown Influence of Role Models: In the context of SLT, determining the impact of a specific role model on an individual’s aggressive behaviour is challenging. It isn't easy to quantify the influence of a role model and separate it from other factors in a person's life. Moreover, the number of exposures necessary for behaviour to be imitated is not clearly defined, adding another layer of complexity. Variability in Individual Responses: Different individuals may respond differently to the same stimuli or role models due to personal, psychological, and situational factors. This individual variability makes it challenging to draw broad conclusions from specific studies. However, the broader question concerns how social learning influences aggressive behaviour in real-life contexts. Some non-experimental studies seem to support Bandura's theories:

MEDIA VIOLENCE ON TELEVISION: Research indicating a spike in violent attacks following major boxing events suggests a potential link between the observation of violence in media and aggressive behaviour in real life. This is in line with the concept of social learning, where witnessing violence could lead to imitative aggressive behaviour.

Bandura’s Snake Phobia Experiment (1961): In this study, Bandura demonstrated how children could overcome the fear of snakes through observational learning. Children who observed a model interacting fearlessly with a snake were more likely to overcome their phobias, showing the power of social models in changing behaviour.

Charlton et al.’s Study on Children in St. Helena (1999): Investigating the introduction of television to the island of St. Helena, this study found that despite increased exposure to media violence, there was no corresponding increase in aggressive behaviour among children. This provided insights into the complexity of how media violence affects behaviour and the moderating role of community and parental influences.

Liebert & Baron’s Study on Television and Aggression (1972): This study found that children who watched violent episodes of a police drama exhibited more aggressive play afterwards than those who watched a non-violent sporting event, supporting the idea that observed behaviour can influence actions.

Walters & Thomas’ Research on Delinquent Adolescent Boys (1963): This study showed that delinquent boys often imitate aggressive and criminal behaviours observed in family members, supporting SLT’s premise that behaviour can be learned through observation of close models.

Huesmann et al.’s Study on TV Viewing and Aggression (2003): A longitudinal study found that childhood exposure to TV violence predicted aggressive and violent behaviours in adulthood, suggesting long-term effects of observed behaviours during childhood

DRUG ADDICTION Addiction (Vaping, Smoking, and Illicit Drugs):

Akers et al.’s Social Learning Theory of Alcohol and Drug Behavior (1979): This study applied SLT to alcohol and drug use, suggesting that substance use behaviours are learned through the same social processes as other behaviours. The study emphasized the role of peer influence, imitation of drug-using behaviours observed in others, and reinforcement of substance use through social interactions.

Kandel’s Research on Peer Influence in Adolescent Drug Use (1978): Denise Kandel's work on adolescent drug use highlighted the significant role of peer groups in the initiation and continuation of drug use. Her research demonstrated that adolescents are more likely to start using drugs when they have friends who are drug users, consistent with SLT's emphasis on the influence of observational learning and peer modelling.

EATING DISORDERS

The Fiji Television Study (Becker et al., 2002): This landmark study investigated the impact of the introduction of Western television on body image and eating attitudes in Fijian adolescent girls. It found that after television exposure, there was a significant increase in symptoms of eating disorders. The study highlights how exposure to media and the consequent observational learning of body ideals can influence eating behaviours and attitudes.

Family Influence on Eating Disorders (Cooley et al., 2018): Research has shown that family dynamics and parental attitudes towards weight and diet can influence the development of eating disorders in children. Observational learning within the family context, such as imitating parents' eating habits or internalizing their attitudes towards food and body image, plays a significant role.

Peer Influence and Body Image (Jones et al., 2004): This study explored how peer groups influence body dissatisfaction and eating disorders. The findings suggested that individuals often compare themselves to their peers and may adopt disordered eating behaviours that they observe within their social circles.

Gender Roles and Gender Critical Theory: Bussey and Bandura (1999): In their study on gender role socialization, Bussey and Bandura expanded on SLT to explain how children acquire gender-typed knowledge and behaviours. They emphasised the role of modelling and reinforcement in learning gender roles, showing how children imitate the behaviour of same-sex models and how parents and peers differentially reinforce their gender-typed behaviours.

Fagot (1985): Fagot’s study on parental reactions to children's behaviour showed that parents often reinforce gender-stereotypical play and penalize cross-gender activities. This research provided empirical support for SLT in the context of gender role development, demonstrating how parents' responses to children's behaviour can reinforce societal gender norms.

Martin, Wood, and Little (1990): This study focused on the early gender role development in preschool children. It highlighted how children as young as three years old have well-established gender stereotypes and prefer gender-typed toys, which they likely learned through observational learning and reinforcement. The Impact of Television on Gender Role Development: Several studies have focused on how television and media exposure influence children's perception of gender roles. For instance, research by Williams et al. (1986) on television and children's gender role socialisation found that children who watch a lot of television have more stereotypical views of gender roles, likely due to the gendered behaviours and roles often depicted in media content.

However, it is important to note that most of the above studies are not experiments but rather observational or correlational. This is a common approach in research on gender roles and social learning, as it often involves studying natural behaviours in real-world settings, which are not easily manipulated in an experimental design. Correlational and do not establish a cause-and-effect relationship. While they support the idea that exposure to violence in media might be associated with increased aggression, they cannot conclusively prove that watching violence directly causes aggressive behaviour. For example, it could be that the reason that aggressive ramps up in boxing matches is because of alcohol and not vicariously being reinforced by boxing. Thus, other factors might also contribute to the observed increase in violence, and these factors must be considered when interpreting the results.

However, some experimental studies have been conducted in this area.

Bussey and Bandura (1999): Conducted experiments on children's learning of gender behaviours from models. Media Influence Studies: Experiments showing children gender-typical or atypical media content and observing changes in attitudes or behaviours. Educational Setting Experiments: Research in schools to see how different gender role modelling affects children's attitudes and behaviours. Parental Behaviour Experiments: Studies where adults' responses to children's gendered behaviours are manipulated to observe effects on the children. Though less common due to ethical and practical challenges, these studies provide experimental evidence on gender roles and social learning.

DETERMINISM: The deterministic aspect of Social Learning Theory (SLT) in explaining aggression suggests that individuals are predestined to display aggressive behaviour based on their environmental exposures and learning experiences, particularly through observation and imitation. This perspective implies that behaviour is largely shaped by external factors, often without consciously evaluating the actions' consequences or moral considerations. Unconscious Learning: SLT posits that much of behaviour is learned unconsciously. In the context of aggression, individuals might learn aggressive behaviours by observing others, especially significant figures like parents, without actively thinking about or understanding the implications of such behaviour. Role of Environment: According to SLT, the environment plays a crucial role in shaping behaviour. If an individual is exposed to aggressive models, such as parents who exhibit aggressive behaviour or media that glorifies violence, they are more likely to learn and replicate such behaviour. This learning occurs through observation and imitation, often subconsciously. Absence of Individual Blame: A deterministic view through the lens of SLT tends to shift blame away from the individual and towards the environment and external influences. This perspective suggests that behaviour results from conditioning and environmental factors rather than intrinsic characteristics or choices. Influence of Parenting and Media: SLT underscores the importance of parental behaviour and media exposure in developing aggression. Parents who display aggressive behaviour may inadvertently teach their children to be aggressive. Similarly, unrestricted exposure to adult media that portrays violence can also serve as a learning model for aggressive behaviour. Implications for Punishment and Responsibility: Understanding aggression as a learned behaviour affects how society addresses and punishes such behaviour. If aggressive actions are viewed as learned rather than inherently chosen, it might influence the approach to rehabilitation and punishment. This perspective could focus on altering environmental factors and providing new learning experiences to modify behaviour rather than solely punitive measures. In conclusion, the deterministic view of SLT on aggression emphasises the significant role of the environment and external influences in shaping behaviour. It suggests that individuals learn aggression through observation and imitation of their surroundings, often without conscious awareness, shifting the focus from individual culpability to environmental and societal factors in addressing aggressive behaviour.

THE IMPACT OF TV: Numerous discussions have arisen around the impact of televised violence on children's behaviour. While several studies suggest that television does not necessarily provoke aggression in children, concerns have been raised about the violent content in some cartoons. Psychologists have observed instances where such cartoons may lead to aggressive behaviours in children. Despite this, the general public often perceives these cartoon interactions, like Jerry the mouse hitting Tom the cat, as comical and entertaining. Consequently, it is often seen as the duty of parents to educate their children about the fictional nature of these cartoons. R.D. Singer conducted a notable study that challenges common perceptions about the impact of television violence on children's behaviour. Over six weeks, Singer observed two groups of juvenile boys: one group was exposed to violent television content, while the other watched non-violent shows. Contrary to what many might expect, the findings revealed that the boys who viewed non-violent content exhibited more aggressive behaviour compared to their counterparts who watched violent shows. The results of Singer's study suggest that watching violent television can reduce aggression in viewers. This phenomenon is explained by the concept of the "Catharsis effect." According to this theory, viewing violent scenes on television allows viewers to empathize with the characters involved in the aggression. Through this empathetic engagement, viewers are theorized to vicariously release their aggressive thoughts and feelings. As a result, the act of watching violence on television serves as a form of emotional release, leading to a decrease in their aggressive tendencies. This finding presents a counterintuitive perspective on the impact of media violence, proposing that it can have a therapeutic, rather than inciting, effect on aggression. Cooke proposed that people often endorse the idea that television violence leads to aggression as a means to rationalize the aggressive behaviours they observe in society. He also viewed television as an educational tool and a source of positive role modelling. Following this logic, if violence on television can incite aggression, then surely the virtuous traits portrayed on TV should encourage kindness and good behaviour among viewers. Thus, television has the potential to act as a deterrent by emphasizing positive characteristics. Despite such critiques, Albert Bandura's Social Learning Theory is a significant framework for

understanding aggression and criminal behaviour. Bandura emphasised the role of family and media in presenting positive role models, both to children and the wider public, as a strategy for curbing aggressive tendencies (Bandura, 1976)

CHILDREN DON’T EXIST IN A VACUUM: No individual, particularly a child, exists in a vacuum. Their behaviours, preferences, and development are influenced by many factors, often interacting in complex ways. While significant, the impact of watching TV or playing violent games is just one piece of a much larger puzzle. For instance, neglected children may watch excessive TV to stimulate or escape. In such scenarios, the content of what they watch, including exposure to violence or adult themes, becomes a concern. However, it's crucial to understand that this behaviour might be symptomatic of deeper underlying issues, such as neglect, lack of positive role models, or absence of constructive engagement in their lives. In these cases, watching too much TV or engaging in certain media consumption is not the root cause but rather a manifestation of other underlying problems. These could include Neglect: Neglected children might not receive adequate parental guidance or supervision, leading them to seek alternative sources of

or comfort. Lack of Boundaries: Children might be exposed to content unsuitable for their age, impacting their understanding of the world and behaviour. Absence of Positive Role Models: Children learn by observing and imitating. If they lack positive role models, they might turn to characters seen in media as substitutes who may not always exhibit desirable behaviours. Lack of Philosophical or Moral Guidance: Discussions about right and wrong, ethical dilemmas, and moral reasoning are crucial in a child’s development. Without this guidance, children might struggle to contextualise or understand the content they are exposed to in media. Therefore, while media consumption, like watching TV or playing video games, is often scrutinized for its impact on children’s behaviour, it is important to consider it within the broader context of a child's overall environment and experiences. Addressing the root causes, such as neglect or the absence of positive influences, is key to fostering healthier development and well-being in children.

HORIZONTAL EVOLUTION: The Social Learning Theory (SLT) can be likened to a form of "horizontal evolution" in cultural and behavioural development. This analogy is particularly pertinent when considering the rapid progress of Homo sapiens and other primates like chimpanzees. In traditional Darwinian evolution, change occurs vertically through genetic inheritance over generations. Traits and behaviours that offer survival advantages are passed down from parents to offspring. This process, while fundamental to the development of species, is relatively slow, as it relies on genetic variations and natural selection over many generations. In contrast, horizontal evolution relates to SLT and involves spreading ideas, behaviours, and knowledge across individuals within the same generation. This horizontal transmission happens much faster than genetic changes. Through observation, imitation and learning from others, individuals can acquire new behaviours and skills without waiting for genetic changes across generations. This form of social learning is a key driver in human societies' rapid cultural and technological advancements. It allows for the swift dissemination of innovations, such as tool use, language, and social norms; in primates like, observational learning leads to the spread of certain behaviours within a group, contributing to

cultural evolution. The concept of horizontal evolution through SLT provides a compelling explanation for the rapid progress of human civilizations. It highlights how learning from others and sharing knowledge can lead to swift changes and adaptations in a population, far outpacing the rate of biological evolution. This perspective underscores the importance of social learning and cultural transmission in evolving complex behaviours and societal structures.

MIRROR NEURONS: Social Learning Theory (SLT) and the concept of mirror neurons is closely related to learning, empathy, and understanding others' emotions. Mirror neurons are a type of brain cell that respond equally when we perform an action and witness someone else perform the same action. This discovery has significantly impacted our understanding of social behaviour and learning. In the context of SLT, mirror neurons might provide the neurological basis for observational learning – the cornerstone of Bandura’s theory. When we observe someone acting, our mirror neurons activate similarly as we are acting ourselves. This neural mirroring helps us understand and imitate the actions of others, facilitating the learning process. Moreover, mirror neurons are thought to play a crucial role in empathy. When we see someone experiencing an emotion, such as pain or joy, our mirror neurons for those emotions are activated, allowing us to "feel" what the other person is feeling. This empathetic response is essential for social understanding and emotional connection. An interesting implication of this is seen in studies involving Botox. Botox, which paralyzes facial muscles to reduce wrinkles, can also dampen the ability to mimic others' facial expressions. Since mimicry is a key part of how mirror neurons function, there's speculation that Botox might reduce our ability to empathize. This is based on the theory that if you can't mimic someone's pain or happiness on your face due to Botox, the mirror neurons might not activate as effectively, potentially leading to a decrease in empathetic response over time. This idea intersects intriguingly with SLT, as both the theory and the function of mirror neurons emphasize the importance of observation and mimicry in learning and understanding social cues. The concept that physical alterations like Botox could influence our neural processes and, subsequently, our social learning and empathy highlights the complex interplay between our biology and social experiences

POSITIVE ASPECTS AND CONTRIBUTIONS OF SLT: Despite the earlier criticisms, Bandura's research has significantly contributed to our understanding of observational learning, particularly in aggressive behaviour. His work underscores two key aspects: the ability of individuals, especially children, to learn through observation and the role of vicarious consequences in determining whether observed behaviours are imitated. Key Contributions of Bandura's Research: Learning through Observation: Bandura's experiments, most notably with the Bobo doll, demonstrate that individuals can learn and adopt behaviours simply by observing others. This challenged the prevailing behaviourist notion that all learning results from direct reinforcement or punishment. Vicarious Reinforcement and Punishment: Bandura's work shows that people do not need to experience the consequences of a behaviour personally to learn from it. Observing the consequences that others experience can be enough to encourage or discourage the observer's behaviour.

ROLE MODELS The Social Learning Theory (SLT), as developed by Bandura, extends beyond understanding aggressive behaviours. It has profound and positive applications in various fields like therapy, child management, teaching, and parenting. The essence of SLT – learning through observation – underscores the significance of providing children with positive role models. For instance, this could mean therapists demonstrating healthy coping strategies or positive social interactions, which clients, especially children, can observe and learn from. Teachers can embody and display behaviours that promote constructive social interactions and effective conflict resolution in educational settings. Parents, too, can harness the principles of SLT by being conscious role models for their children, understanding that their young ones' actions, responses, and attitudes are likely to be mirrored

Further extending into the broader cultural context, SLT reminds us of the impact of public figures and media on behaviour and societal trends. An example of this is the influence of celebrities like Kim Kardashian, whose popularity has significantly driven trends such as the desire for larger posteriors, leading to an increase in procedures like Brazilian butt lifts. This phenomenon illustrates the power of 'negative role models' and the capacity of media figures to shape societal norms and personal aspirations, often in ways that can have profound health and psychological implications. Therefore, Bandura's SLT provides invaluable insights not only into understanding behavioural acquisition but also into guiding effective strategies in therapy, education, and parenting, emphasizing the critical role of role models – both positive and negative – in shaping behaviours and societal trends.

CONCLUSION: To conclude, exploring aggression through the lens of Social Learning Theory presents a range of ethical concerns, methodological obstacles, and the intricate nature of human behaviour influenced by numerous factors. These challenges highlight the necessity for careful analysis of research outcomes in this domain and emphasise the significance of adopting a comprehensive strategy to comprehend

Previous
Previous

THE ETHOLOGICAL EXPLANATION OF AGGRESSION

Next
Next

THE FRUSTRATION AGGRESSION HYPOTHESIS