SIVYER PSYCHOLOGY

View Original

COGNITIVE EXPLANATIONS FOR SCHIZOPHRENIA

Cognitive explanations of schizophrenia focus on understanding how faulty cognitive processes—such as attention deficits, memory problems, and disorganised thinking—contribute to the disorder's symptoms. They provide a theoretical framework that describes how these cognitive impairments may lead to both positive and negative symptoms. In contrast, Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) is a practical therapeutic approach that applies these cognitive insights to help patients manage their symptoms. While cognitive explanations aim to explain why the symptoms occur, CBT focuses on changing maladaptive thought patterns and behaviours to improve the individual's quality of life. Essentially, cognitive explanations provide the understanding, while CBT offers the treatment

Cognitive explanations, including dysfunctional thought processing.

A01 THEORY: BREAKDOWN OF PERPETUAL FILTERING

THE COGNITIVE APPROACH TO SCHIZOPHRENIA

The cognitive approach primarily focuses on developing theoretical models that simulate mental processes in a way that mirrors how the biological brain functions. Cognitive psychologists believe that internal thought processes can be understood by breaking them down into their individual components, such as memory, attention, perception, and language. For example, understanding how memory works alongside language processing gives insight into how the brain operates as a system.

In the context of mental illness, cognitive explanations focus on identifying which parts of this "mental machine" are not functioning correctly. When we apply this to schizophrenia, cognitive explanations suggest that the disorder arises due to faulty cognitive processes—the parts of the brain that manage memory, attention, perception, and language are not working as they should. As profound thought disturbances characterise schizophrenia, cognitive psychologists suggest that disturbed thinking processes are the cause rather than the consequence of schizophrenia.

Many cognitive theories about the development of schizophrenia share the assumption that cognitive impairments—such as poor memory control, language difficulties, and disorganised thinking—play a key role in both the development and maintenance of the disorder. In other words, schizophrenia can be viewed through the lens of faulty cognitive processes that lead to the symptoms patients experience.

In contrast to neurotypical individuals, people with schizophrenia are unable to filter distractions or distinguish between relevant and irrelevant information. They are highly sensitive to all kinds of stimuli—both from the external environment and internal thoughts—and struggle to integrate these perceptions into a coherent pattern. In other words, they cannot sustain focus, filter out unnecessary stimuli, or prioritise incoming and outgoing information. This cognitive overload results in disorganised and overwhelming sensations and thoughts, which significantly affect their concentration. Consequently, individuals with schizophrenia become easily distracted by everything around them, which is often reflected in their speech and behaviour. For a person with schizophrenia, their brain's filtering system is essentially broken, leading to the characteristic cognitive difficulties associated with the disorder

Individuals with schizophrenia also often struggle with concentration and find it difficult to maintain order in their thoughts and show a significant reduction in problem-solving and decision-making abilities. As a result, much research has focused on the role of attention in schizophrenia.

A01 THEORY: BREAKDOWN OF PERCEPTUAL FILTERING (FRITH'S ATTENTION DEFICIT THEORY)

One prominent cognitive explanation for schizophrenia is Frith's Attention Deficit Theory, which suggests that people with schizophrenia have a breakdown in their ability to filter stimuli. This theory proposes that the cognitive mechanisms responsible for filtering and prioritising information are impaired in individuals with schizophrenia. This breakdown affects their ability to focus on relevant information while ignoring irrelevant or distracting stimuli, leading to disorganised thoughts and difficulty concentrating.

A01 RESEARCH: BREAKDOWN OF PERCEPTUAL FILTERING

Research supports this theory by demonstrating that individuals with schizophrenia perform worse on laboratory tasks requiring them to focus on certain stimuli while ignoring others. For instance, studies such as those by McGhie and Chapman (1961) found that people with schizophrenia struggled significantly with tasks requiring selective attention. They were unable to screen out irrelevant stimuli, leading to difficulties in concentration and organisation of thought.

Further studies, such as those by Venables (1964), have shown similar deficits in perceptual filtering. Venables found that individuals with schizophrenia were more easily distracted by external stimuli and performed poorly in tasks requiring attention control compared to neurotypical individuals.

These findings indicate that the cognitive filtering system is defective in individuals with schizophrenia, supporting the notion that they experience a breakdown in perceptual filtering processes. This difficulty in sorting and prioritising incoming information contributes to the cognitive symptoms observed in the disorder, such as disorganised thinking and impaired attention.

A03 RESEARCH ANALYSIS: BREAKDOWN OF PERPETUAL FILTERING

  • Cognitive theories of attention and perception are well-established and backed by scientific research.

  • There is evidence contradicting the breakdown of the filtering explanation. Studies of brain-damaged patients have shown that they often experience similar cognitive deficits to schizophrenics, such as problems with attention or with the relationship between memory and perception—for example, H.M. and the patients studied by Schmolck et al.. However, although these patients have cognitive deficits, they don't show the symptoms of schizophrenia. This challenges the cognitive explanation. This means that the cognitive explanation alone is unlikely to be valid as if it was these brain-damaged patients would develop schizophrenic symptoms. Therefore, other factors, such as biochemistry, must play a role in the cause of schizophrenia.

  • The faulty attention system and self-monitoring explanation account for the positive symptoms of schizophrenia but not the negative symptoms.

FRITH'S THEORY (A01)

Frith (1992) proposed a cognitive explanation for schizophrenia, with a focus on deficits in Theory of Mind (ToM), which is the ability to understand and attribute mental states—beliefs, desires, intentions—to oneself and others. Frith suggested that individuals with schizophrenia struggle to monitor their own mental states as well as the thoughts and intentions of others. This impairment in ToM could explain many of the positive symptoms, such as delusions and hallucinations, and negative symptoms, such as withdrawal and apathy, observed in schizophrenia.

Additionally, Frith broadened his explanation by including deficits in self-monitoring, attention, and perceptual filtering. He suggested that schizophrenics have a failure to filter irrelevant information and manage their attention, which results in disorganised thinking and an inability to interpret the world in a coherent manner. For example, they may struggle to distinguish external stimuli from internal thoughts, contributing to their confusion and fragmented reality.

A03 RESEARCH ANALYSIS OF FRITH'S THEORY

There is supporting evidence for Frith's explanation of schizophrenia, particularly regarding Theory of Mind (ToM). For example, Drury, Robinson, and Birchwood found that individuals with schizophrenia had difficulty interpreting the beliefs and intentions of others, which lends credence to the idea that they have deficits in meta-representation and ToM. This difficulty in understanding others' mental states can explain why people with schizophrenia often misinterpret social cues, leading to delusional beliefs.

However, alternative interpretations suggest that the poor performance on ToM tasks in these individuals may not necessarily indicate a lack of ToM. It could be due to an information-processing overload caused by their difficulty filtering stimuli and focusing attention. This aligns with Frith’s suggestion that problems with attention and information filtering contribute to the disorder, but it indicates that cognitive impairments might not be limited to ToM. This raises the possibility that deficits in attention or working memory play a larger role in the cognitive dysfunctions observed in schizophrenia.

In summary, while Frith’s Theory of Mind hypothesis provides valuable insight into schizophrenia, it is likely that additional cognitive factors, such as information filtering and attention deficits, also play a critical role in the disorder. These findings suggest that a multifactorial cognitive approach is necessary to fully understand the cognitive breakdowns in schizophrenia.

FRISTON'S THEORY (A01)

Friston (1995) introduced a neurobiological perspective on schizophrenia, focusing on the disorganisation of cognitive processes, particularly during auditory hallucinations. He hypothesised that in normal individuals, internal thoughts progress into internal language through a feedback loop that helps them recognise that their inner speech is self-generated. This feedback loop ensures that the person knows their internal voice is their own. However, in individuals with schizophrenia, this feedback loop is disrupted, leading to auditory hallucinations, where they mistake their own inner voice for an external one.

Friston also proposed that schizophrenia involves a breakdown in the synchronisation of brain regions. For example, the frontal lobes, responsible for planning and intentions, and the temporal lobes, responsible for language processing, fail to communicate properly. This disconnection leads to fragmented thoughts and perceptions, where the individual cannot accurately connect their actions to their perceptions. Consequently, they may attribute their inner thoughts or actions to external forces, which can explain the paranoid delusions often observed in schizophrenia.

Additionally, Friston suggested that this breakdown in self-awareness and cognition could explain why many individuals with schizophrenia have religious or supernatural delusions. The disconnect between their thoughts and reality may lead them to interpret their experiences through culturally significant frameworks, such as hearing voices of God or spirits.

A03 RESEARCH ANALYSIS OF FRISTON'S THEORY

There is research evidence that supports Friston’s theory regarding the misinterpretation of internal speech in schizophrenia. For instance, McGuigan (1966) found that individuals with schizophrenia often mistake their inner speech for someone else’s voice. His study showed that their vocal cords were active during auditory hallucinations, suggesting that they were speaking internally but interpreting it as external speech. This supports Friston's idea that the feedback loop responsible for distinguishing internal from external speech is disrupted in schizophrenia.

Furthermore, this explanation is consistent with Hemsley's theory of poor integration between memory and perception, which also highlights a breakdown in the cognitive processing of sensory information. This reinforces Friston's argument that there is a disconnection between different brain regions responsible for thought organisation and perception.

Despite these findings, some critics argue that while Friston’s theory explains certain positive symptoms (like hallucinations and delusions), it may not fully account for the negative symptoms of schizophrenia, such as avolition and social withdrawal. These negative symptoms may require a more comprehensive neurobiological model that also considers the role of dopamine dysregulation or glutamate abnormalities.

In conclusion, Friston’s theory provides a compelling explanation for the fragmented perceptions and hallucinations observed in schizophrenia, but a broader framework that integrates both cognitive and biochemical factors may be necessary to fully understand the disorder.

STRENGTHS AND LIMITATIONS

One of the significant strengths of cognitive explanations for schizophrenia is their ability to account for both the positive symptoms (such as hallucinations and delusions) and the negative symptoms (such as emotional withdrawal and lack of motivation). Many other models, including the dopamine hypothesis, Gottesman’s meta-analysis, and Bateson's Double Bind Theory, have largely focused on explaining positive symptoms. For instance, the dopamine hypothesis primarily addresses the excess dopamine activity linked to hallucinations and delusions, while Gottesman’s genetic research and Bateson’s theory of communication breakdowns also focus more on these overt symptoms.

Cognitive explanations provide a more comprehensive understanding by linking both positive and negative symptoms to underlying dysfunctions in cognitive processes, such as attention, memory, and perception. For example, cognitive deficits like self-monitoring impairments (the ability to recognise one’s own thoughts as self-generated) can explain auditory hallucinations, while deficits in attention and motivation account for the negative symptoms like apathy. This makes the cognitive approach a valuable tool for clinicians and researchers, as it offers a more holistic view of schizophrenia.

However, despite the detailed descriptions of cognitive impairments, the cognitive approach is not a complete explanation of schizophrenia. While it identifies cognitive deficits, it does not explore the root causes of these deficits. It remains unclear whether these cognitive dysfunctions are a cause of schizophrenia or a consequence of the disorder. Cognitive models are often based on correlational data, which means that while they show a relationship between faulty cognition and symptoms, they cannot definitively prove causality. This is similar to the criticisms faced by the dopamine hypothesis, where increased dopamine activity is linked to symptoms of schizophrenia, but causality remains uncertain.

INTEGRATION WITH BIOLOGICAL EXPLANATIONS

Interestingly, cognitive explanations also provide insight into some of the limitations of purely biological theories, such as the dopamine hypothesis. While antipsychotic medications can correct dopamine imbalances, it often takes days or weeks for positive symptoms to fade. This delay may suggest that cognitive processes, such as faulty self-monitoring and poor attention, are deeply ingrained and persist as a kind of "bad habit" even when dopamine levels are restored to normal. This highlights the importance of integrating cognitive and biological explanations, rather than treating them as separate or competing theories.

Frith’s (1992) work supports this integration by linking cognitive deficits to biological abnormalities. Frith found that individuals with schizophrenia have reduced blood flow to areas of the brain associated with dopamine regulation, such as the prefrontal cortex. This region is responsible for managing attention and self-monitoring—two key cognitive processes impaired in schizophrenia. These findings support the idea that cognitive dysfunctions are not purely psychological but are tied to biological factors in the brain. In this way, the cognitive and biological models complement each other, offering a more complete explanation of schizophrenia's complex nature.

COGNITIVE THERAPY AND PATIENT PREFERENCES

Cognitive therapy offers a valuable alternative to patients who may be hesitant to use or want to reduce their reliance on antipsychotic medication. Unlike drug therapies, which target biological symptoms, cognitive therapies aim to help individuals understand their thought patterns and develop strategies to cope with symptoms. This can be especially appealing to patients who feel that medication alone is not addressing their full range of experiences.

Morrison et al. (2014) found that dropout rates for cognitive therapy were lower than for drug therapies, and cognitive therapy was equally effective at reducing psychotic symptoms. This suggests that some patients may find cognitive therapy more engaging or manageable, offering them an empowering alternative to medications that can have unpleasant side effects.

THE RECOVERY MODEL AND THE HEARING VOICES MOVEMENT

Cognitive explanations also align with the principles of the Recovery Model, which encourages individuals with schizophrenia to view their experiences in a less pathological light. Rather than simply trying to eliminate symptoms, the Recovery Model promotes understanding and managing symptoms like hearing voices. One prominent example is the Hearing Voices Movement, which advocates for helping people with schizophrenia make sense of their voices and develop coping strategies, rather than simply viewing these experiences as symptoms to be medicated away.

Eleanor Longden, a key figure in this movement, highlights this approach in her well-known TED talk, where she explains how learning to understand and manage her voices helped her regain control of her life. This non-pathologising perspective is an important part of cognitive therapy, as it shifts the focus from treating schizophrenia as a purely biological illness to an experience that can be understood and managed through empowerment and personal insight.

CONCLUSION

In conclusion, while cognitive explanations of schizophrenia provide a detailed understanding of the disorder’s cognitive impairments, they must be integrated with biological explanations to offer a complete picture. Cognitive therapy, rooted in these explanations, provides an important treatment alternative, especially for those who prefer a non-medical approach to managing their symptoms. Moreover, the cognitive approach supports more holistic perspectives like the Recovery Model, offering individuals with schizophrenia a path towards understanding and coping with their experiences, rather than focusing solely on symptom suppression.