SIVYER PSYCHOLOGY

View Original

NEURAL AND HORMONAL MECHANISM IN THE CONTROL OF EATING BEHAVIOUR

Neural and hormonal mechanisms involved in the control of eating behaviour, including the role of the hypothalamus, ghrelin and leptin

 This unit is about what affects our appetite, i.e. what makes us hungry and satiated/full. In short, it is about our feeding behaviour. REMEMBER, it is biological NEURAL explanations we are looking for. NEURAL means in the brain.

KEYWORDS

Ghrelin

Homeostasis

Hypothalamus both Lateral Hypothalamus (LH) and Ventromedial Hypothalamus (VMH)

Parvo-ventricular nucleus

Insulin

Glucose

Setpoint

Leptin

Duel centre model of feeding

Aphagia (undereating)

Hyperphagia (overeating)

Satiation

Homeostasis: if this did not occur, we would starve or eat to death. Knowing if we are full (our set point) is individual, however. Each individual has a set point, and their weight is regulated around it.

 The stomach’s role in appetite

This theory can only be used if you mention in the A02 how food triggers neural responses in the brain. Remember, this topic is about neural mechanisms, so you won't get a mark if you don’t link the theory to the brain. Simple, right?

Theory: Cannon (1927) believed the stomach governed hunger and satiety. When full, it would stretch and tell the brain to stop eating. 

Research: One of his co-workers, Washburn, swallowed a balloon and inflated it inside his stomach.  As predicted, this stretched the stomach, and he no longer felt hungry.  He used a balloon because they could control for extraneous variables.  If Washburn had used food, then the food could have affected appetite, e.g., Glucose being released from carbohydrates and curbing hunger.

However, this is a huge oversimplification. Patients who have had parts of their stomachs surgically removed still report feelings of hunger and satiety despite nothing to stretch or contract, as do cancer sufferers. They were still able to gauge/regulate their appetite despite having lost parts of their stomachs through surgery. So appetite has to be more than about the stomach being full or empty, then!

Surgically removing the stomach for medical reasons has little effect on maintaining normal body weight (Wangensteen & Carlson, 1931). The same thing has been found experimentally in rats. The only thing that changes is the eating pattern: many small meals instead of a few larger ones.

Furthermore, the stomach contractions Cannon measured were more likely induced by the balloon that measured them than hunger pangs. Washburn failed to detect the stomach contractions reliably even when they did occur (Davis et al., 1959).

Plus, it's a rubbish study anyway—not scientific at all! There was one participant, so it is a case study, DC, subjective, placebo, etc., which is unethical and dangerous.

Canon was not completely wrong, though. The stomach does play a part in appetite. When the stomach is empty, for example, it produces Ghrelin, Which makes us feel hungry.

The current view states that signals from the stomach primarily regulate the time between meals. Several other lines of research support this idea. The time between meals depends on the size of the preceding meal. The larger the meal, the longer the time till the next meal (Le Magnen, 1985). The stomach is still digesting and producing Glucose—high levels of Glucose stop hunger. So the stomach and its fullness play a role, just not in the way Cannon theorised.

Food (caloric) content in the stomach, not just bulk, is important in stopping eating. Putting salt solution, non-digestible cellulose ["fibre"], or other non-nutritive (~without calories) bulk in the stomach stops eating only briefly. If food is diluted with non-nutritive fibre or clay, rats compensate by eating more (Levison et al., 1975).

The Hypothalamus’ role in appetite

The hypothalamus is a portion of the brain that contains several small nuclei with various functions. It is located below the thalamus, just above the brain stem. All vertebrate brains contain a hypothalamus, roughly the size of an almond in humans. The hypothalamus controls body temperature, hunger, thirst, fatigue, and circadian cycles.

The theory on the Hypothalamus

There has been considerable research evidence to support neural mechanisms' role in hunger and satiety. 

 Studies examining this structure's lesioning regions have supported the hypothalamus's role. In 1920, W. R. Hess showed that the hypothalamus regulates body processes, including eating and drinking. (Hess won the Nobel Prize in 1949 for his pioneering work. The current view states that the hypothalamus is the major site for detecting signals of nutritional state and organising hunger-related behaviour. It gets information about the body's need for food through neural connections and hormones from the gut, and it controls food-related responses, partly through the autonomic nervous system and the pituitary gland. A long winding route led to this view.

 Early research showed that damage in the hypothalamus severely disrupts normal eating and regulation of body weight. Damage to the lateral (~side) hypothalamus greatly depresses eating. (Teitelbaum & Epstein, 1962). In contrast, damage to the ventromedial hypothalamus (ventro- = ~bottom or belly side; medial = [toward the] middle) greatly increases eating. A rat with such a lesion can overeat enough to double its normal body weight.

Research on the Hypothalamus

The first study on the role of the hypothalamus was done by Hetherington and Ranson in 1942. Their study involved lesioning rat Hypothalamus. The lesion is cutting away areas of the brain. In this case, it was either the lateral hypothalamus (LH) or the Ventromedial Hypothalamus (VMH). If you forget the part of the hypothalamus, you can say when parts of the Hypothalamus are removed. The results from this study show that the Hypothalamus has an ‘on’ and ‘off’ command for eating. The LH functions as the hunger centre. If this area is lesioned in rats, they starve themselves. If, however, the VMH is lesioned, then they overeat until death. It is suggested that if these areas are dysfunctional, they can lead to eating disorders such as Obesity and Anorexia.

Area of hypothalamus

Effect of lesioning

Effect of stimulating

Ventromedial hypothalamus (satiety centre)

Increases eating

Decreases eating

Lateral hypothalamus (hunger centre)

Decreases eating

Increases eating

Tumours in the VMH have led to excessive binge eating in humans. Hypothalamic obesity is an unfortunate complication in some survivors of brain tumours, especially those diagnosed in childhood. Robert H. Lustig, 2003 ‘The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism’. Damage to the VMH from surgery has long been known to promote excessive eating (hyperphagia) and weight gain, termed "hypothalamic obesity." Victims of this form of obesity continue to gain weight despite their best efforts. De Araujo et al, 2006.

Implanting electrodes into rats' brains (hypothalamus) so they can record the electrical activity of the feeding cycle. So when a rat is hungry, do certain brain regions activate and vice versa for when the rat is full up? Yes, they do.

Electrical stimulation of the hypothalamus. Activity is recorded when electrodes are implanted into the brain and used to stimulate neurons artificially. The findings are the same as above, e.g., VMH inhibits feeding, and LH stimulation encourages it.

Prader-Willi syndrome is frequently associated with an extreme and insatiable appetite, often resulting in morbid obesity. There is currently no consensus on the cause for this particular symptom, although genetic abnormalities in chromosome 15 disrupt the normal functioning of the hypothalamus. Given that the hypothalamus regulates many basic processes, including appetite, there may be a link.

However, no organic defect of the hypothalamus has been discovered in post-mortem investigation.

 Hypothalamus only

The hypothalamus model has been discarded because early studies gave an incomplete picture of the effects of hypothalamic functioning. There is no other evidence that hormones such as Leptin and Glucose are also critically involved in eating behaviour (see below).

In addition, the model, as described, also fails to indicate how we know when to start and stop eating. What are the signals we use? Again it I thought Glucose plays a vital role here. For example, what triggers the hypothalamus to stop and start?

The hypothalamus has other important arrears in appetite, such as the parvo-ventricular nucleus.

Conflicting Evidence Studies such as those involving lesions to the LH and VMH in rats have supported the role of the hypothalamus in regulating eating behaviour. However, Gold (1973) found that lesions restricted to the VMH alone did NOT result in hyperphagia and only produced overeating when they included other areas such as the parvo-ventricular nucleus! However, subsequent research has failed to replicate Gold’s findings, so it is a type 1 error.

Linking site of damage to behaviour: Lesions are when brain areas are cut away. In the past, little was known about the brain, so researchers tended to chop away lots, not realising the effect they were having (think Lobotomy/psycho surgery). But as Logue (2004) points out, destroying brain tissue (even a small lesion) however carefully, could affect far more neurons and cell bodies whose axons or dendrites run through the lesioned area e.g., not just neurons at that site but possibly pathways travelling through. Even the destroyed neurons have connections to many other neurons, which will be affected by the damage. Likely, some of the neurons with cell bodies in the VMH and LH could have connections to the frontal cortex that relay sensory information about food, e.g., smells, sights, motivation to obtain food, etc. For example, it is thought that lesions to the VMH cause damage to the Dopamine system" (Berridge & Robinson, 1995). The Dopaminergic system passes through the lateral and ventromedial hypothalamus. See point 4

Therefore, when researchers lesion the hypothalamus, they damage other areas, affecting behaviour. Unfortunately, researchers concluded that the resulting behaviour was due only to the hypothalamus and not the other areas they had lobbed off.  For example, more complete information shows that damage to the hypothalamus systems VMH and LH produces under- and overeating indirectly. Damage to the lateral hypothalamus depresses responsiveness to all external stimuli, not just food-related ones. These rats sit and do nothing as if completely unmotivated. This finding has led researchers to hypothesise that LH Lesions Cause Sensory Neglect. Marshall et al. (1971) found that LH lesions produce a range of sensory and motor impairments termed sensory neglect.

Rats with LH lesions

  • Do not respond to touch, auditory, or visual stimuli

  • Remain immobile unless disturbed

  • Do not right themselves when placed on their side

Marshall et al.'s findings imply that LH lesions may reduce arousal. Eating is triggered by external stimuli such as the time of day, or the sight and smell of food. If LH lesions interfere with an animal's ability to process such stimuli, this could account for reduced food intake. This explanation casts doubt that the LH is specifically involved in food intake; all behaviour would be affected if sensation was disrupted. The sensory neglect produced by LH lesions is now thought to be caused by the lesion-destroying axons in the Dopamine system or, more specifically, the nigrostriatal pathway.  The nigrostriatal tract utilizes dopamine (DA) as its transmitter. Incidentally, Parkinson's disease is associated with a massive reduction in Dopamine.

The result of this disruption to our Dopamine system is that even if tasty food is put into the mouth, these rats chew and eat it actively. They appear to like the food but don't do anything to get it. They don't because the dopaminergic (dopamine-using) "wanting/reward system" (Berridge & Robinson, 1995) that passes through the lateral hypothalamus is damaged.

These animal experiments appear to correspond to the observation that obese humans eat fewer peanuts if they have to shell them than people of normal weight.

The same is true of rats with VMH lesions. They are initially able to eat loads more, but the motivation of rats to obtain food is less clear-cut (i.e., they will eat ravenously if food is in front of them, but will they be motivated to look for it?). VMH lesioned rats were put through various reinforcement feeding schedules, varying from pressing a lever once to obtain a pellet of food right through to pressing a lever 256 times to obtain one pellet of food. One would imagine that rats with legions to the VMH would be so hungry they would be motivated to press a lever any number of times to get food.

Findings: rats with VMH lesions are more likely to press the lever in the one-tap condition than non-VMH rats, but once the schedule got tougher, they were less motivated than non-VMH lesioned rats to press the lever for food pellets.

Chemical Lesions of the LH We have seen that electrical lesions of the LH have failed to provide clear-cut answers to the role of the LH in feeding. Chemical lesions, on the other hand, did resolve the issue of sensory neglect. Recently, research has employed chemical lesions to destroy cells in the LH whilst sparing axons of the nearby nigrostriatal/dopamine tract. Kainic acid and ibotenic acid lesions of cells in the LH produce a long-lasting decrease in food intake and body weight but do not produce sensory neglect. However, it is widely acknowledged that it is a gross oversimplification to call the LH a 'feeding centre' because:

  • Animals recover from LH lesions.

  • LH lesions disrupt aggression, sexual behaviour, and reinforcement.

  • Nigrostriatal/Dopamine lesions produce aphagia and adipsia

9) a body of evidence indicates that VMH-lesioned rats are pernickety eaters. Compared to normal rats, they eat less when:

·      Quinine is added to make food taste bitter

·      Food is stale

These are not the behaviours you might expect if the VMH is a hunger centre whose destruction increases an animal's motivation for food! The hypothalamus is an insufficient explanation alone. Other biological mechanisms are involved.

Another hypothesis is that damage to the ventromedial hypothalamus produces over-eating and obesity by making gut reflexes work faster. This speeds digestion and removes food faster (Keesey & Powley, 1975). Therefore, the next meal comes much sooner than normal, which produces overeating and obesity.

In any case, the effects of the lesions in the VMH are not permanent. After an initial increase in weight, body weight stabilizes in Rats. VMH lesioned rats could reach satiety (not overeat) despite the absence of their satiety centre. For example, after lesions in the lateral hypothalamus, rats ignore food completely. They will starve to death amid food. They must be force-fed for up to several weeks before they start eating again. Eventually, they recover enough to maintain their body weight with tasty, easily eaten-food, but they keep it below normal. After regaining normal body weight by force-feeding, they eat less until they return to the low weight they had stabilized after the brain lesion. Therefore, there must be other mechanisms involved in appetite.

Pinel has provided a complementary explanation for the role of the VMH in feeding. He suggests that animals with VMH lesions “overeat because they become obese ". He argues that VMH lesions increase blood insulin levels. This triggers lipogenesis (the conversion of glucose into fat). Because any food the animal eats is rapidly converted into fat, they suffer an energy deficiency. Consequently, they eat in vain to overcome the lack of glucose circulating in their bloodstream.

Role of the LH & VMH - What do contemporary textbooks say? The search for a role for the LH in hunger continues. It may play a role in controlling insulin release. According to this idea, excitation in the LH stimulates insulin release from the pancreas. Lesioning the LH is followed by a reduction in insulin release. Insulin allows glucose to be broken down to provide a source of energy. The body compensates for the lack of glucose-derived energy by breaking down stored body fat to provide fuel. Due to fat breakdown, body weight declines. But the blood is full of excess energy-rich free fatty acids, and the animal fails to eat because its body is telling the brain areas controlling feeding that there is no need to take in more energy (Schneider & Tarshis, Elements of Physiological Psychology, McGraw Hill, 1995, p 386-7, 1995).

For those having trouble with the complexity of this topic. Please see the general A02 at the end. Ensure you mention the methodologies used. Most of it is experimental and very good; say why. Please talk about the fact that a variety of methods are good, however are not replicable, the tumor studies done on humans is not as generalisable as tumour grow differently and impact the LH and VMH in different ways, yet at least we have humans, this backs up animal research which lacked comparability to humans.

 Rounding it all up/Conclusions

Dual Centre Model of feeding

 Now that you understand the role of LH, VMH, Glucose, and Insulin, I can tell you about the dual-centre feeding model. This is a SET POINT theory; it suggests we have set points for hunger and satiety. I could, by the way, tell you so much more about this topic; there are many things we have not even discussed, but if you know the Dual Centre model, that should be enough.

 Dual Centre Model of Feeding (DCMF)

Explanation of DCMF

When blood glucose levels fall, the hypothalamus senses this, and the LH is activated. We feel hungry and eat. As food intake is likely to increase blood glucose, the VMH senses this, activating us into feeling full and not eating any more.

Dual Centre Model of Feeding

Set-point theories are rigid and insufficient to explain all aspects of eating behaviour. For instance:

Early ancestors needed to eat a lot to store energy as fat.

Eating behaviour is not always motivated by an energy deficit. Other factors, such as taste, learning, and society, influence eating behaviour.

Does not account for the influence of hormones such as ghrelin, Leptin, etc.

The latest theory and the best theory on appetite  ….

.Neural and hormonal signals of the body's energy balance come from the gut and fat storage cells in many body parts. Two kinds of signals from the gut reach the hypothalamus: hormones and neural signals. The main hormones are insulin, which controls the level of blood glucose ("blood sugar"), and leptin, which fat cells release when they are filled. The neural signals come from receptors in the gut that detect stomach content and hormones the gut releases in response to food level. One of these is CCK, a hormone the gut releases when it is filled with food. The neural signals get to the central nervous system mainly over the vague nerve in the parasympathetic division of the autonomic nervous system. This large nerve connects the insides of the body to the brain stem.

Leptin, insulin, and blood glucose levels regulate food intake in the short term (hours). Leptin is also important in long-term body weight regulation because it reflects calories stored in fat cells.

Low insulin levels and leptin signal the body is in a negative energy balance (using more energy than it is getting). These signals depress unnecessary body activity to conserve energy and activate eating to counteract this condition. High insulin levels and leptin signal that the body is in a positive energy balance (getting more energy than it uses). These signals increase body activity to burn energy and inhibit eating in this condition. In summary, insulin and leptin levels generate negative feedback to counteract both low and high levels of food to keep the body within homeostatic limits. The basic physiological core that regulates eating does not operate in isolation. Many psychological, social, and cultural processes modify eating. These processes include associative learning, food variety, food palatability (tastiness), visual appearance, social setting for eating, and cultural effects.

 Research and theory applicable to all the above theories

 Reductionism

We can see that biological theories see appetite as caused by only one factor. They exclude other approaches, yet there are individual differences in appetitesAppetites are probably influenced by various factors: genetics, family, culture and society. Therefore, biological reductionism is not a good way to understand appetites, i.e., appetite cannot just be boiled down to biological concepts; it probably can be better understood with a few approaches. Moreover, there is substantial and convincing evidence that social, cultural and psychological factors affect our eating behaviours, as is evident from psychological explanations of eating disorders.

Ecological Validity

Highly controlled lab experiments with humans may lack mundane realism and ecological validity, as in Cannon’s study.

 Cannot extrapolate/generalise animal findings to humans:

Against: Firstly, many animals, such as rats, have eating behaviours and eating patterns that are different from humans. Also, it is unclear whether the influences are completely the same for humans, though, as our eating is not just geared (like many mammals) as an automatic stimulus–response drive. In humans, Cognition/mood/culture may also play a part in our eating habits. Food and over/under eating also have a social significance to us, in how we look, are perceived as greedy, etc.

Also, research on animals may not be directly comparable as we are physiologically different despite our many similarities. Thus, research on animals has resulted in huge medical blunders for humans: thalidomide, Opren, etc. Moreover, animals have different reactions to different foods. For example, dogs cannot tolerate chocolate, and Guinea pigs are allergic to penicillin.

Many researchers would counter-argue that rodents have more similarities than dissimilarities. For example, our lower-down brain structures (hypothalamus, hindbrain and forebrain) are the same. Plus, we all produce the same (hormones: insulin, Ghrelin, Leptin).

Against Animal Ethics.  

Lesioning is an invasive brain technique. Is it unnecessarily cruel to lesion animals, they probably die or starve overeat to death. Plus, there are caging and anaesthesia issues.

 For animal ethics

Are there any psychological/medical advances, especially in understanding the causes of obesity (Heart attacks, diabetes, strokes, and shorter life spans by nine years) is a very current problem (There are currently 1.6 billion overweight adults in the world, according to the World Health Organization- WHO). That number is projected to grow by 40% over the next ten years). Anorexia and Bulimia Nervosa are also areas where the end may justify the means.

This theory (biological) is deterministic because it says you have no free will over your appetite set point. Appetite is determined by biology. This cannot be true as people do have some success at dieting. Surely dieting indicates free will (though it has to be said that most people fail at dieting)? Can physiological drives be overridden (e.g. desire to lose weight, dislike of certain foods, fear of losing control, social cues to continue eating, food availability)?

Physiological drives can be overridden (e.g. desire to lose weight, dislike of certain foods, fear of losing control, social cues to continue eating, food availability.

Such studies provide sound scientific evidence. The experiments on rats were well-controlled and scientific. Rats do not suffer from individual differences the way humans do.

One weakness of the model is that it ignores the behavioural approach. This means that focusing solely on biology (the hypothalamus) ignores the role that learning plays in feeding behaviour. For example, research has shown that we may learn to associate certain times of the day with eating, so we feel hungry at these times through classical conditioning. This problem suggests that learning also plays a role in eating behaviour, so a solely biological approach may be too narrow.

Biological rhythms impact eating behaviour. We tend to feel hungry at certain times of the day, regardless of what we’ve eaten earlier. Rats tend to feel hungry just after darkness. This suggests a link between the body clock and eating mechanisms, but again, the precise link isn’t known, though the main clock is located in adjacent areas of the hypothalamus.