SIVYER PSYCHOLOGY

View Original

CAREGIVER INFANT REACTIONS

CAREGIVER-INFANT INTERACTIONS IN HUMANS:

SPECIFICATION: RECIPROCITY AND INTERACTIONAL SYNCHRONY

The specification highlights Reciprocity and Interactional Synchrony specifically so you can probably get questions that directly target this (if not a full essay, smaller questions are possible too). If a “discuss research into” type question comes up you can use research studies for AO1 (highlighted in blue) - Its possible a 12 marker may ask this however if the question is outline/explain - Do not use the studies I have listed below in blue! They will not count for AO1.

INTRODUCTION

When we refer to infancy, this is generally understood to mean the first year of a child’s life and the period before they begin to learn to speak. One of the main key interactions between caregivers and infants is their non-verbal communication, for example, how they communicate without words or sounds.

These interactions are believed to form the basis of attachment between the caregiver and the infant. How each responds to one another through how sensitive they are to one another’s signals determines the formation of attachment and how deep the relationship is.

Andrew Meltzoff video

RECIPROCITY

Reciprocity occurs between infants and carers resulting in mutual behaviour where both parties can generate a response from one another almost like a conversation e.g smiling by the parent results in smiling by the baby. Such interactions between the infant and carer facilitate and strengthen the attachment bond. Brazelton (1979) suggested reciprocal behaviour was an important precursor for later communication development. Signals the infant gives allow the carer to anticipate the child’s responses and respond appropriately. This sensitivity to the child’s needs and behaviour lays the foundation for developing attachment. Tronick et al. (1979) found that when mothers who had been engaged in dialogue with their babies were asked to stop moving and remain static, the babies would become puzzled and distressed when their smiles could not provoke a reciprocal response. This highlights how babies engage and anticipate reciprocal responses to their behaviour.

INTERACTIONAL SYNCHRONY

Interactional synchrony involves infants imitating specific hand and facial gestures from an adult model. It broadly refers to a finely tuned coordination of behaviours between the child and parent during speaking and listening. Infants and parents are seen to develop a shared sense of timing and rhythm, which develops into a flow of mutual behaviours. Meltzoff & Moore (1983) demonstrated that interactional synchrony occurred with infants imitating facial expressions, tongue protrusions and mouth openings from an adult model when only three days old. This suggests the behaviour was innate rather than learned.

THE DIFFERENCE BETWEEN RECIPROCITY AND INTERACTIONAL SYNCHRONY

The difference between reciprocity and interactional synchrony lies in the nature and timing of the interactions between caregivers and infants.

RECIPROCITY refers to a mutual exchange between caregiver and infant, where both parties take turns in responding to each other's signals. This back-and-forth interaction is crucial for developing communication skills and emotional bonding. Reciprocity can be seen when an infant cries and the caregiver responds by comforting them or when a caregiver speaks and the infant coos in response. The key aspect of reciprocity is the responsiveness to each other's cues, fostering a sense of connection and understanding.

INTERACTIONAL SYNCHRONY involves a more synchronized and simultaneous sharing of emotional states and behaviors between the caregiver and infant. This phenomenon is characterized by the mirroring of facial expressions, movements, and vocal sounds in a coordinated way. Interactional synchrony is more about the 'dance' of nonverbal communication, often leading to a harmonious connection between the caregiver and the infant. This synchronous interaction helps to establish a secure attachment relationship and supports the infant's social and emotional development.

In summary, while reciprocity is about the turn-taking interactions that build communication and bonding, interactional synchrony focuses on the simultaneous mirroring and sharing of behaviours and emotions, creating a deep and harmonious bond between caregiver and infant.

RECIPROCITY AND INTERACTIONAL SYNCHRONY RESEARCH

FELDMAN AND EIDELMAN (2007): This study by Feldman and Eidelman delved into the realm of interactional synchrony, discovering that mothers and their infants, as young as three months old, exhibited moments of synchrony that significantly predicted the infant's social and emotional development. These synchronous interactions were linked to enhanced problem-solving skills and more securely attached relationships in later infancy, highlighting synchronised behaviours' critical role in early developmental outcomes.

EDWARD TRONICK ("Still Face Experiment"): Edward Tronick's research, particularly through the "still face experiment," provided pivotal insights into interactional synchrony. Although the experiment primarily showcased infants' distress when faced with a lack of responsive interaction, it also implicitly emphasized the necessity of synchronous exchanges for fostering emotional well-being and a secure bond between the infant and caregiver.

MELTZOFF AND MOORE (1977) This study is the key to reciprocity and interactional synchronicity.

Meltzoff and Moore's study conducted in 1977 stands as a pivotal investigation into the dynamics of reciprocity and interactional synchrony within caregiver-infant interactions. This groundbreaking research highlighted infants' innate ability to imitate adults' facial expressions and gestures, demonstrating that such mirroring behaviour begins much earlier. Through a series of controlled experiments, Meltzoff and Moore observed that infants as young as a few weeks old could mimic specific facial movements and hand gestures made by adults, such as sticking out the tongue or opening the mouth. This mimicry suggests a fundamental form of communication and connection between caregivers and infants, underscoring the importance of these early non-verbal interactions in social and emotional development. The findings from Meltzoff and Moore's study have significantly contributed to our understanding of the early stages of human development, emphasizing the role of innate mechanisms in fostering caregiver-infant bonding and communication through reciprocity and synchronous interactions.

CONDON AND SANDER (1974) One of the seminal studies in the area of reciprocity meticulously analyzed frame-by-frame video recordings of infants and observed that even newborns participate in interactional synchrony, moving in precise time with the structure of adult speech. This early form of reciprocity, where infants respond to speech patterns with coordinated movements, underscores the innate capacity for social interaction.

ISABELLA ET AL. (1989) found a correlation between secure attachment and interactional synchrony behaviours within the first year of life. Infants who demonstrated more synchronized behaviours with their caregivers were more likely to be classified as securely attached. This suggests that the quality of early nonverbal communication between infants and caregivers, through mechanisms like interactional synchrony, plays a crucial role in developing attachment security.

BRAZELTON ET AL. (1974). Another pivotal researcher introduced the "reciprocity dance" concept t” to describe the rhythmic and turn-taking nature of interactions between mothers and their infants. They highlighted how these early exchanges are critical in developing communication skills as infants learn to read and respond to social cues.

Overall, the research on reciprocity highlights its vital role in the early stages of human development, emphasizing how these initial social exchanges lay the groundwork for complex communication, emotional regulation, and social skills.

RECIPROCITY AND INTERACTIONAL SYNCHRONY EVALUATION

One significant challenge in studying caregiver-infant interactions is the difficulty in accurately testing infant behaviour, given the constant fluctuation of facial expressions. The behaviours observed in the MELTZOFF & MOORE (1977) study, such as sticking their tongue out, yawning, smiling, opening their mouths, and hand movements, are common occurrences in young babies. This frequent variability in expressions and actions presents a substantial obstacle in clearly differentiating between mere general behaviour and genuine instances of interactional synchrony. Consequently, theories derived from such observations may struggle with internal validity issues, as they might not precisely measure interactional synchrony. This complexity underscores the inherent challenges in validating studies focused on the nuanced interactions between caregivers and infants, potentially questioning the accuracy of interpretations regarding synchronous behaviour.

MELTZOFF & MOORE’s (1977) study also faces issues with reliability, as subsequent attempts to replicate their findings have not always been successful. For instance, KOEPKE ET AL. (1983) could not replicate the same outcomes, casting doubt on the consistency of the original study's results. Meltzoff and Moore acknowledged a significant limitation in their research, noting that their study lacked sufficient control, which could affect its validity. This admission highlights potential methodological weaknesses that might contribute to difficulties reproducing the observed effects of interactional synchrony, thereby questioning the robustness of their findings within the broader research community.

Despite the above flaws, there is much support for the theoretical frameworks behind them. Studies offer empirical support for theoretical frameworks in developmental psychology, particularly attachment theory. These studies demonstrate how early non-verbal communication patterns like reciprocity and interactional synchrony contribute to developing secure attachments.

The findings of LE VINE ET AL. (1994), highlight an important aspect of attachment theory's application across different cultures. Le Vine and colleagues' research in Kenya, which observed that Kenyan mothers had relatively little interactive or face-to-face engagement with their infants compared to what is often observed in Western societies, yet still had a high proportion of securely attached children, suggests that the manifestations of caregiver-infant attachment can vary significantly across cultural contexts.

This challenges the notion that specific behaviours like interactional synchrony are universally required to develop secure attachment. While interactional synchrony may play a significant role in some cultural contexts, it is not a universal requirement for developing a secure attachment relationship. Instead, secure attachment can emerge from various caregiving behaviours that meet the infant's needs, interpreted within their society's cultural norms and practices.

Therefore, while interactional synchrony has been observed and studied primarily within Western contexts, Le Vine et al. (1994) and similar cross-cultural studies indicate that attachment security is not solely dependent on this specific form of interaction. These insights contribute to a broader understanding of attachment as a flexible construct that can adapt to fit different societies' cultural practices and values, underscoring the importance of considering cultural context in developmental psychology research.

One practical application of these findings on interactional synchrony and the importance of caregiver-infant interactions is the recommendation for mothers to be placed in the same room with their newborns immediately after birth. This practice facilitates the early formation of attachment bonds, marking a significant shift from previous practices where mothers and their infants were kept separate. Such immediate contact supports the development of a strong, secure attachment by leveraging the natural tendencies for interactional synchrony, allowing both mother and child to engage in mutual, responsive behaviours from the beginning. This approach fosters emotional bonding and encourages positive developmental outcomes for the infant. DEYONG ET AL. (1991) conducted a study where infants were observed interacting with two objects: one simulating tongue movements and the other simulating the opening and closing of the mouth. Their findings revealed that infants, within the median age of 5 to 12 weeks, showed little interactional synchrony or response to these objects. This suggests that infants display specific social responses to human interactions, supporting the concepts of reciprocity and interactional synchrony by indicating that infants do not simply imitate any observed action. Instead, their responses are more discerning, implying a preference or innate ability to engage more significantly with human beings rather than non-animate objects. This distinction underscores the importance of human-to-human interaction in the early development of social behaviours and the formation of attachment bonds.

METHODOLOGICAL CHALLENGES IN OBSERVATIONS: Research in this area often relies on observational studies, which can be subjective. The interpretation of interactional synchrony or reciprocity might vary, leading to potential biases. Moreover, these dynamic and fluid interactions make them difficult to quantify accurately.

Integrating neuroimaging techniques in studies on caregiver-infant interactions, particularly in the context of reciprocity and interactional synchrony, has added depth and objectivity to our understanding. These methodologies allow researchers to observe the neural correlates of these interactions, providing concrete evidence of their impact on the brain's development and functioning.

EXAMINATION QUESTIONS

  • What is meant by the term interactional synchrony? (2 marks)

  • What is meant by reciprocity? (2 marks)

  • Outline one study of infant-caregiver interactions (4 marks)

  • Discuss infant-caregiver interactions referring to reciprocity and interactional synchrony in your answer (12 marks AS, 16 marks A-level).Caregiver-Infant Interactions In Humans